skip to main content

Meet the people who never forget a face

"Being good at recognising a face doesn't necessarily mean someone is also good at face matching." Photo: Getty Images
"Being good at recognising a face doesn't necessarily mean someone is also good at face matching." Photo: Getty Images

Analysis: while some of us struggle to recognise faces of friends and family, "super-recognisers" possess exceptional face recognition skills

By Ryan Jenkins, University of Greenwich; David James Robertson, University of Strathclyde and Josh P Davis, University of Greenwich

How good do you think you are at recognising faces? What about voices? Some people are great at it, while others struggle. Researchers have found a large variation in people’s abilities to recognise the faces or voices of those completely unknown to them.

When it comes to face recognition, the ability ranges from those who struggle to recognise the faces of friends and family – a condition known as "prosopagnosia" or face-blindness – to those who have demonstrated exceptional unfamiliar face recognition skills, called "super-recognisers". These super-recognisers have been successfully deployed in many agencies, including London’s police service.

From BBC's One Show, how UK police forces are using super-recognisers

Similar findings have also been discovered for voice recognition. The condition "phonagnosia" describes those who struggle to recognise the voices of their friends and family. But whether or not someone can possess exceptionally good voice recognition ability remained uncovered, until now.

In a new study, we and our colleagues tested whether super-face-recognisers could transfer their abilities across to recognising voices, to explore the possibility of a super-voice-recogniser.

Face and voice abilities

Typically, to assess the ability levels of super-recognisers, researchers have often used two tests. First, the Cambridge face memory test measures the ability to learn and remember a face. Then the Glasgow face matching test is used to measure the ability to say whether two faces belong to the same person or two different people.

However, being good at recognising a face doesn’t necessarily mean someone is also good at face matching. Research has shown even super-recognisers can be very good at face memory, but just as good as typical ability participants on face matching or vice versa.

We need your consent to load this rte-player contentWe use rte-player to manage extra content that can set cookies on your device and collect data about your activity. Please review their details and accept them to load the content.Manage Preferences

From RTÉ Radio 1's Ryan Tubridy, interview with author Henrietta McKervey about her book Violet Hill and Padraig O'Riordan, who works as a super-recogniser with London's Metropolitan Police.

Voice tests have also been designed, not to measure super-recognition skills, but rather to measure the general ability to remember a voice , and to decide whether two voices belong to the same person or two different people. But the extent to which super-recognisers can perform well on voice tests was not yet examined.

What our study found

In our recent study, we tested four groups of participants, based on face memory and face-matching abilities. The participants completed a test of voice memory, a test of voice matching and a test for identifying celebrity voices. The study had a number of findings.

First, we found voice recognition ability varies substantially beyond the definitions found in current literature, which describes people falling into two categories, either "typical" or phonagnosic. We found people can perform very well at voice recognition, beyond the typical range abilities.

Second, we found those who possessed exceptional face memory skills, face-matching skills, or both, outperformed those with typical ability skills at voice memory and voice matching.

A woman smiling with a green box drawn around her face.

The research provides evidence to suggest individuals with excellent voice-recognition abilities may be able to enhance policing and security operations. Photo: Shutterstock/metamorworks

Some participants managed to achieve consistently high scores across multiple tests. This hints at the possibility of a super-voice-recogniser. However further research is needed to properly substantiate this possibility.

Partly this is because the voice tests used were never originally designed to distinguish between the exceptional and the very good, so perhaps are unable to fully explore superior voice processing. As such, new voice tests specifically designed to focus on the upper end of the voice-recognition ability spectrum are required.

Our research provides tentative support to the idea that there may be some link between different mechanisms in the brain. These could be cross-modality (voices and faces) and cross-task (memory and perception) mechanisms that, working together, drive this kind of superior ability to recognise voices and faces.

From Good Mythical Morning with Rhett & Link, are you a super-recogniser?

It may be the case that these individuals can be useful in similar job positions that super-face-recognisers have also been employed in, such as police and security forces. This is particularly relevant when voice clips are the only available evidence, such as in cases of phone surveillance, kidnapping, fraud, blackmail and counter-terrorism operations.

Our work is the first to explore the potential abilities of super-voice-recognisers and ask whether those that possess exceptional face memory abilities, face-matching abilities or both can transfer their skills across to voice tests.

It also provides the first piece of work to suggest individuals with excellent voice-recognition abilities may be able to enhance policing and security operations. Screening for those with such an ability might be a useful tool during recruitment stages of these types of professions.The Conversation

Ryan Jenkins is a PhD Candidate in Psychology at the University of Greenwich. David James Robertson is a Lecturer in Psychology at the University of Strathclyde. Josh P Davis is a Reader in Applied Psychology at the University of Greenwich. This article was originally published by The Conversation.


The views expressed here are those of the author and do not represent or reflect the views of RTÉ